Results for 'A. De Houwer'

963 found
Order:
  1.  35
    Evaluative Learning with “Subliminally” Presented Stimuli.Jan de Houwer, Hilde Hendrickx & Frank Baeyens - 1997 - Consciousness and Cognition 6 (1):87-107.
    Evaluative learning refers to the change in the affective evaluation of a previously neutral stimulus that occurs after the stimulus has been associated with a second, positive or negative, affective stimulus . Four experiments are reported in which the AS was presented very briefly. Significant evaluative learning was observed in participants who did not notice the presentation of the affective stimuli or could not discriminate between the briefly presented positive and negative ASi when asked to do so . In two (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  2.  25
    Toward a cumulative science of emotion: a functional-cognitive framework for emotion research.Jan De Houwer & Sean Hughes - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (1):61-66.
  3. The propositional nature of human associative learning.Chris J. Mitchell, Jan De Houwer & Peter F. Lovibond - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (2):183-198.
    The past 50 years have seen an accumulation of evidence suggesting that associative learning depends on high-level cognitive processes that give rise to propositional knowledge. Yet, many learning theorists maintain a belief in a learning mechanism in which links between mental representations are formed automatically. We characterize and highlight the differences between the propositional and link approaches, and review the relevant empirical evidence. We conclude that learning is the consequence of propositional reasoning processes that cooperate with the unconscious processes involved (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   85 citations  
  4.  39
    On the generality of the affective Simon effect.Jan De Houwer, Geert Crombez, Frank Baeyens & Dirk Hermans - 2001 - Cognition and Emotion 15 (2):189-206.
    In affective Simon studies, participants are to select between a positive and negative response on the basis of a nonaffective stimulus feature (i.e., relevant stimulus feature) while ignoring the valence of the presented stimuli (i.e., irrelevant stimulus feature). De Houwer and Eelen (1998) showed that the time to select the correct response is influenced by the match between the valence of the response and the (irrelevant) valence of the stimulus. In the affective Simon studies that have been reported until (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  5.  46
    The relational responding task: toward a new implicit measure of beliefs.Jan De Houwer, Niclas Heider, Adriaan Spruyt, Arne Roets & Sean Hughes - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:132367.
    We introduce the Relational Responding Task (RRT) as a tool for capturing beliefs at the implicit level. Flemish participants were asked to respond as if they believed that Flemish people are more intelligent than immigrants (e.g., respond “true” to the statement “Flemish people are wiser than immigrants”) or to respond as if they believed that immigrants are more intelligent than Flemish people (e.g., respond “true” to the statement “Flemish people are dumber than immigrants”). The difference in performance between these two (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  6. RANDELL, Tom (2004): Robust affective priming effects in a conditional pronunciation task: Evidence for the semanticrepresentation of evaluative information.Jan de Houwer - 2004 - Cognition and Emotion 18 (2).
  7.  36
    Do smokers have a negative implicit attitude toward smoking?Jan De Houwer, Roel Custers & Armand De Clercq - 2006 - Cognition and Emotion 20 (8):1274-1284.
  8.  7
    Putting relating at the core of language-of-thought.Jan De Houwer - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e272.
    Propositional representations are units of information with a relational content. Their relational nature allows for the six distinctive properties of language-of-thought representations. Putting relating at the core of language-of-thought also fits well with the idea that thinking and reasoning are instances of relational behavior. These propositional and behavioral perspectives can be combined within a functional-cognitive framework.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  20
    What is intuiting and deliberating? A functional–cognitive perspective.Jan De Houwer, Yannick Boddez & Pieter Van Dessel - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e117.
    We applaud De Neys for drawing attention to the interaction between intuiting and deliberating without committing to single- or dual process models. It remains unclear, however, how he conceptualizes the distinction between intuiting and deliberating. We propose several levels at which the distinction can be made and discuss the merits of defining intuiting and deliberating as different types of behavior.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10. A structural analysis of indirect measures of attitudes.Jan De Houwer, J. Musch & K. C. Klauer - 2003 - In Jochen Musch & Karl C. Klauer (eds.), The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion. Lawerence Erlbaum.
  11.  12
    Beyond evaluative conditioning? Searching for associative transfer of nonevaluative stimulus properties.J. De Houwer - 2005 - Cognition and Emotion 19 (2):283-306.
    Evaluative conditioning refers to the changes in liking of an evaluatively neutral stimulus (the conditional stimulus or CS) as a result of merely pairing it with another, already liked or disliked stimulus (the unconditional stimulus or US). We examined whether other, non‐evaluative stimulus properties of a US can also be associatively transferred to a CS. In a series of experiments, we tried to transfer perceptions of the gender of children and the gender of first names. We found evidence for the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  12.  55
    An Affective Variant of the Simon Paradigm.Jan De Houwer & Paul Eelen - 1998 - Cognition and Emotion 12 (1):45-62.
    In this paper, we introduce anaffective variant of the Simon paradigm. Three experiments are reported in which nouns and adjectives with a positive, negative, or neutral affective meaning were used as stimuli. Depending on the grammatical category of the presented word (i.e. noun or adjective), participants had to respond as fast as possible by saying a predetermined positive or negative word. In Experiments 1 and 2, the words POSITIVE and NEGATIVE were required as responses, in Experiment 3, FLOWER and CANCER (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  13.  38
    Self-esteem revisited: Performance on the implicit relational assessment procedure as a measure of self- versus ideal self-related cognitions in dysphoria.Jonathan Remue, Jan De Houwer, Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Marie-Anne Vanderhasselt & Rudi De Raedt - 2013 - Cognition and Emotion 27 (8):1441-1449.
  14.  32
    Hooked on a feeling: affective anti-smoking messages are more effective than cognitive messages at changing implicit evaluations of smoking.Colin Tucker Smith & Jan De Houwer - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  15.  49
    A time course analysis of the affective priming effect.Dirk Hermans, Jan De Houwer & Paul Eelen - 2001 - Cognition and Emotion 15 (2):143-165.
    The argument that automatic processes are responsible for affective/evaluative priming effects has been primarily based on studies that have manipulated the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA; i.e., the interval between the onset of the prime and the onset of the target). Moreover, these SOA studies provide an insight in the time course of the activation processes underlying automatic affect/attitude activation. Based on a fine-grained manipulation of the SOA employing either the evaluative decision task (Experiment 1) and the pronunciation task (Experiment 2) (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  16.  15
    (1 other version)Corrigendum: Environmentally Sustainable Food Consumption: A Review and Research Agenda From a Goal-Directed Perspective.Iris Vermeir, Bert Weijters, Jan De Houwer, Maggie Geuens, Hendrik Slabbinck, Adriaan Spruyt, Anneleen Van Kerckhove, Wendy Van Lippevelde, Hans De Steur & Wim Verbeke - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
  17.  23
    Automatic appraisal of motivational valence: Motivational affective priming and Simon effects.Agnes Moors & Jan De Houwer - 2001 - Cognition and Emotion 15 (6):749-766.
    We investigated whether motivationally determined stimulus valence can be processed in an automatic way, as is assumed in many appraisal theories (e.g., Frijda, 1986, 1993; Lazarus, 1991; Scherer, 1993a). Whereas appraisal theorists typically use conscious self-report methods to investigate their assumptions, our experiments used indirect experimental methods that leave less room for deliberate, conscious reflections of the participants. Using variants of the affective priming and Simon paradigms, we demonstrated that intrinsically neutral, but wanted stimuli facilitated responses with a positive valence, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  18.  52
    Evaluative conditioning effects are modulated by the nature of contextual pairings.Sean Hughes, Yang Ye & Jan De Houwer - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 33 (5):871-884.
    ABSTRACTAcross two studies participants completed a learning phase comprised of two types of trials: context pairing trials in which two words were identical or opposite to one another and evaluative conditioning trials in which a CS was paired with a US. Based on the idea that EC occurs because CS-US pairings function as a symbolic cue about the relation between the CS and the US, we hypothesised that the nature of context pairings might moderate EC effects. Results indicate that identity-based (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  19.  14
    Behavioral Reluctance in Adopting Open Access Publishing: Insights From a Goal-Directed Perspective.Massimo Köster, Agnes Moors, Jan De Houwer, Tony Ross-Hellauer, Inge Van Nieuwerburgh & Frederick Verbruggen - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Despite growing awareness of the benefits of large-scale open access publishing, individual researchers seem reluctant to adopt this behavior, thereby slowing down the evolution toward a new scientific culture. We outline and apply a goal-directed framework of behavior causation to shed light on this type of behavioral reluctance and to organize and suggest possible intervention strategies. The framework explains behavior as the result of a cycle of events starting with the detection of a discrepancy between a goal and a status (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  21
    Contingency learning and unlearning in the blink of an eye: A resource dependent process.James Schmidt, Jan de Houwer & Derek Besner - 2010 - Consciousness and Cognition 19 (1):235-250.
    Recent studies show that when words are correlated with the colours they are printed in, colour identification is faster when the word is presented in its correlated colour than in an uncorrelated colour. The present series of experiments explored the possible mechanisms involved in this colour-word contingency learning effect. Experiment 1 demonstrated that the effect is already present after 18 learning trials. During subsequent unlearning, the effect extinguished equally rapidly. Two reanalyses of data from Schmidt, Crump, Cheesman, and Besner ruled (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  21.  83
    Link-based learning theory creates more problems than it solves.Chris J. Mitchell, Jan De Houwer & Peter F. Lovibond - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (2):230-246.
    In this response, we provide further clarification of the propositional approach to human associative learning. We explain why the empirical evidence favors the propositional approach over a dual-system approach and how the propositional approach is compatible with evolution and neuroscience. Finally, we point out aspects of the propositional approach that need further development and challenge proponents of dual-system models to specify the systems more clearly so that these models can be tested.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22. What is automaticity? An analysis of its component features and their interrelations.Agnes Moors & Jan De Houwer - 2007 - In Agnes Moors & Jan Houweder (eds.), Bargh, John A. (2007). Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes. Frontiers of Social Psychology. (Pp. 11-50). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press. X, 341 Pp.
    Many views define the concept of automaticity in terms of a number of features, but they differ with regard to the features they put most emphasis on, as well as with regard to the coherence they assume among the features. One contemporary account is the gradual and decompositional view, which proposes to investigate each automaticity feature separately and determine the degree to which it is present. In this chapter, we engage in a detailed analysis of the most important features in (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  23.  34
    Failures to induce implicit evaluations by means of approach–avoid training.Katrien Vandenbosch & Jan De Houwer - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (7):1311-1330.
    Woud, Becker, and Rinck (2008) asked participants to repeatedly push pictures of certain faces away and to pull pictures of other faces towards them using a joystick. Performance in a subsequent affective priming task showed that previously pulled faces evoked more positive implicit evaluations then previously pushed faces. We report five studies in which we failed to find consistent evidence for the effect of approach–avoid training on implicit evaluations. We also failed to reproduce the effect reported by Woud et al. (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  24.  35
    Learning, awareness, and instruction: Subjective contingency awareness does matter in the colour-word contingency learning paradigm.James R. Schmidt & Jan De Houwer - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (4):1754-1768.
    In three experiments, each of a set colour-unrelated distracting words was presented most often in a particular target print colour . In Experiment 1, half of the participants were told the word-colour contingencies in advance and half were not . The instructed group showed a larger learning effect. This instruction effect was fully explained by increases in subjective awareness with instruction. In Experiment 2, contingency instructions were again given, but no contingencies were actually present. Although many participants claimed to be (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  25.  30
    Suppressing the truth as a mechanism of deception: Delta plots reveal the role of response inhibition in lying.Evelyne Debey, Richard K. Ridderinkhof, Jan De Houwer, Maarten De Schryver & Bruno Verschuere - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 37:148-159.
  26.  26
    The impact of valenced verbal information on implicit and explicit evaluation: the role of information diagnosticity, primacy, and memory cueing.Pieter Van Dessel, Jeremy Cone, Anne Gast & Jan De Houwer - 2020 - Cognition and Emotion 34 (1):74-85.
    ABSTRACTPrevious research has shown that the presentation of valenced information about a target stimulus sometimes has different effects on implicit and explicit stimulus evaluations. Importantly,...
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  27.  27
    Unreliable Yet Still Replicable: A Comment on LeBel and Paunonen.Maarten De Schryver, Sean Hughes, Yves Rosseel & Jan De Houwer - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  28.  40
    The impact of instruction- and experience-based evaluative learning on IAT performance: a Quad model perspective.Colin Tucker Smith, Jimmy Calanchini, Sean Hughes, Pieter Van Dessel & Jan De Houwer - 2020 - Cognition and Emotion 34 (1):21-41.
    ABSTRACTLearning procedures such as mere exposure, evaluative conditioning, and approach/avoidance training have been used to establish evaluative responses as measured by the Implicit Association...
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  29.  30
    The contextual malleability of approach-avoidance training effects: approaching or avoiding fear conditioned stimuli modulates effects of approach-avoidance training.Gaëtan Mertens, Pieter Van Dessel & Jan De Houwer - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (2):341-349.
    Previous research showed that the repeated approaching of one stimulus and avoiding of another stimulus typically leads to more positive evaluations of the former stimuli. In the current study, we examined whether approach and avoidance training effects on evaluations of neutral stimuli can be modulated by introducing a regularity between the approach-avoidance actions and a positive or negative stimulus. In an AAT task, participants repeatedly approached one neutral non-word and avoided another neutral non-word. Half of the participants also approached a (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  30.  27
    On the role of goal relevance in emotional attention: Disgust evokes early attention to cleanliness.Julia Vogt, Ljubica Lozo, Ernst Hw Koster & Jan De Houwer - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (3):466-477.
    Prior evidence has shown that aversive emotional states are characterised by an attentional bias towards aversive events. The present study investigated whether aversive emotions also bias attention towards stimuli that represent means by which the emotion can be alleviated. We induced disgust by having participants touch fake disgusting objects. Participants in the control condition touched non-disgusting objects. The results of a subsequent dot-probe task revealed that attention was oriented to disgusting pictures irrespective of condition. However, participants in the disgust condition (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  31.  35
    Extinction of likes and dislikes: effects of feature-specific attention allocation.Jolien Vanaelst, Adriaan Spruyt, Tom Everaert & Jan De Houwer - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (8):1595-1609.
    The evaluative conditioning effect refers to the change in the liking of a neutral stimulus due to its pairing with another stimulus. We examined whether the extinction rate of the EC effect is moderated by feature-specific attention allocation. In two experiments, CSs were abstract Gabor patches varying along two orthogonal, perceptual dimensions. During the acquisition phase, one of these dimensions was predictive of the valence of the USs. During the extinction phase, CSs were presented alone and participants were asked to (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32.  24
    Learned helplessness and its relevance for psychological suffering: a new perspective illustrated with attachment problems, burn-out, and fatigue complaints.Yannick Boddez, Pieter Van Dessel & Jan De Houwer - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (6):1027-1036.
    We develop a new perspective on various forms of psychological suffering – including attachment issues, burn-out, and fatigue complaints – by drawing on the construct of learned helplessness. We conceptualise learned helplessness in operant terms as the behavioural effects of a lack of reinforcement and in goal-directed terms as the dysregulation of goal-directed behaviour. Our central claim is that if one fails to reach a goal (e.g. the goal to secure a job), then not only this goal but also other (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  29
    Activation of latent self-schemas as a cognitive vulnerability factor for depression: The potential role of implicit self-esteem.Erik Franck, Rudi De Raedt & Jan De Houwer - 2008 - Cognition and Emotion 22 (8):1588-1599.
  34.  32
    Robust affective priming effects in a conditional pronunciation task: Evidence for the semantic representation of evaluative information.Jan De Houwer & Tom Randell - 2004 - Cognition and Emotion 18 (2):251-264.
  35.  27
    Beyond evaluative conditioning? Searching for associative transfer of nonevaluative stimulus properties.Jan De Houwer, Frank Baeyens, Tom Randell, Paul Eelen & Tom Meersmans - 2005 - Cognition and Emotion 19 (2):283-306.
    Evaluative conditioning refers to the changes in liking of an evaluatively neutral stimulus (the conditional stimulus or CS) as a result of merely pairing it with another, already liked or disliked stimulus (the unconditional stimulus or US). We examined whether other, non‐evaluative stimulus properties of a US can also be associatively transferred to a CS. In a series of experiments, we tried to transfer perceptions of the gender of children and the gender of first names. We found evidence for the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  36. Contingency learning and unlearning in the blink of an eye: A resource dependent process.James R. Schmidt, Jan De Houwer & Derek Besner - 2010 - Consciousness and Cognition 19 (1):235-250.
  37.  61
    How distinctive is affective processing? On the implications of using cognitive paradigms to study affect and emotion.Andreas B. Eder, Bernhard Hommel & Jan De Houwer - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (6):1137-1154.
    Influential theories on affect and emotion propose a fundamental differentiation between emotion and cognition, and research paradigms designed to test them focus on differences rather than similarities between affective and cognitive processes. This research orientation is increasingly challenged by the widespread and successful use of cognitive research paradigms in the study of affect and emotion—a challenge with far-reaching implications. Where and on what basis should theorists draw the line between cognition and emotion, and when is it useful to do so? (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  38.  21
    Persistence and disengagement in failing goals: commentary on Boddez, Van Dessel, & De Houwer.Veronika Brandstätter - 2022 - Cognition and Emotion 36 (6):1042-1048.
    Boddez, Van Dessel, and De Houwer in their paper “Learned helplessness and its relevance for psychological suffering: A new perspective illustrated with attachment problems, burn-out, and fatigue complaints” advance the idea that failing to reach a goal of personal importance unleashes detrimental processes (i.e. learned helplessness) which spill over to other (similar) goals, in the long run resulting in passivity and psychological suffering. As the authors conceptualise learned helplessness in motivational terms (lack of reinforcement, dysregulation of goal-directed response) and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  35
    Induction of implicit evaluation biases by approach–avoidance training: A commentary on Vandenbosch and De Houwer.Marcella L. Woud, Eni S. Becker & Mike Rinck - 2011 - Cognition and Emotion 25 (7):1331-1338.
  40.  32
    Affective and Identity Priming with Episodically Associated Stimuli.Jan De Houwer Dirk Hermans Paul Eelen - 1998 - Cognition and Emotion 12 (2):145-169.
  41. Using the implicit association test does not rule out an impact of conscious propositional knowledge on evaluative conditioning.Jan de Houwer - 2006 - Learning and Motivation 37 (2):176-187.
  42.  49
    What are implicit measures and why are we using them.Jan De Houwer - 2006 - In Reinout W. Wiers & Alan W. Stacy (eds.), Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction. Sage Publications.
  43.  12
    On How Definitions of Habits Can Complicate Habit Research.Jan De Houwer - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
  44.  56
    The Power of Goal-Directed Processes in the Causation of Emotional and Other Actions.Agnes Moors, Yannick Boddez & Jan De Houwer - 2017 - Emotion Review 9 (4):310-318.
    Standard dual-process models in the action domain postulate that stimulus-driven processes are responsible for suboptimal behavior because they take them to be rigid and automatic and therefore the default. We propose an alternative dual-process model in which goal-directed processes are the default instead. We then transfer the dual- process logic from the action domain to the emotion domain. This reveals that emotional behavior is often attributed to stimulus-driven processes. Our alternative model submits that goal-directed processes could be the primary determinant (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  45.  37
    Contingency awareness and evaluative conditioning: When will it be enough?Jan De Houwer - 2001 - Consciousness and Cognition 10 (4):550-558.
  46.  27
    Automatic affective processing.Jan De Houwer & Dirk Hermans - 2001 - Cognition and Emotion 15 (2):113-114.
  47.  23
    Priming (affective).Jan De Houwer - 2009 - In David Sander & Klaus Scherer (eds.), Oxford Companion to Emotion and the Affective Sciences. Oxford University Press.
  48.  29
    Call for Review Paper proposals.Jan De Houwer & Dirk Hermans - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (3):559-559.
  49.  41
    Implicit beliefs about ideal body image predict body image dissatisfaction.Niclas Heider, Adriaan Spruyt & Jan De Houwer - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  50.  51
    Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm.Frederick Verbruggen & Jan De Houwer - 2007 - Cognition and Emotion 21 (2):391-403.
1 — 50 / 963